Відмінності між версіями «N Psychophysiology. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of Topological Psychology. New York, NY»

Матеріал з HistoryPedia
Перейти до: навігація, пошук
м
м
 
(не показано 3 проміжні версії 3 учасників)
Рядок 1: Рядок 1:
Mem. Lang. 49, 201?13. Nadig, A., and Sedivy, J. (2002). Proof of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychol. Navon, D. (1977). Forest just before trees: the precedence of global options in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Compact, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes inside the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). [http://www.testofislam.com/members/frog0faucet/activity/586212/ Und an interaction involving social context and valance. A third possibility] Richardson, D. Nevertheless, other research suggests that empathy is disrupted when we're distracted and cognitively occupied (Gu and Han, 2007). For the reason that attentional sources are usually depleted during everyday interactions, it really is important to know if empathy is automatically engaged or demands controlled and effortful processing. Hence, the existing study examines the role of automaticity and attention in neural processes underlying empathy.CORE NEURAL REGIONS FOR EMPATHYA crucial purpose to appear at empathy for many emotions beneath a number of attentional situations is that it enables for an analysisof core neural regions for empathy. Previous research has identified neural regions that are consistently activated throughout empathy for physical discomfort (i.e., dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, dACC; and anterior insula, AI) (Morrison et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2005; Zaki et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2009; Lamm et al., 2011). These dependable activations inside the dACC and AI have led some researchers to conclude that these regions are a part of a core network in empathy (Fan et al., 2011). Even so, it can be unknown whether the dACC and AI are crucial to empathic processes additional normally (i.e., not only empathy for pain) and whether these regions are activated for the duration of empathy for each good and damaging feelings. Current neuroimaging analysis suggests that other neural regions--such as the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC; BA ten), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC; BA 9), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; BA 11)--may be involved in empathic processes.N Psychophysiology. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of Topological Psychology. New York, NY: McGraw Hill. Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R. A., Bard, E. G. and Jeuniaux, P. (in press). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cogn. Sci. Metzing, C., and Brennan, S. (2003). When conceptual pacts are broken: partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions. J. Mem. Lang. 49, 201?13. Nadig, A., and Sedivy, J. (2002). Proof of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest just before trees: the precedence of global characteristics in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Modest, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes inside the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving collectively: toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Exp. Brain Res. 211, 329?36. Richardson, D. C., and Dale, R. (2005). Wanting to fully grasp:
+
Nadig, A., and Sedivy, J. (2002). Proof of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. [https://www.medchemexpress.com/GSK2656157.html GSK2656157] Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest ahead of trees: the precedence of international attributes in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Tiny, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes within the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving with each other: toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Even though previous study has recommended that specific factors--such as similarity to the target and familiarity with an experience--can trigger empathy (Preston and De Waal, 2002; Mitchell et al., 2006; Xu et al., 2009), really little investigation has examined how interest impacts our potential to empathize. Previous study suggests that empathy may happen instantaneously and automatically when we recognize another's emotional state (Preston and De Waal, 2002), even if we are cognitively busy. On the other hand, other research suggests that empathy is disrupted when we are distracted and cognitively occupied (Gu and Han, 2007). Simply because attentional sources are typically depleted during daily interactions, it is actually significant to understand if empathy is automatically engaged or calls for controlled and effortful processing. Thus, the present study examines the part of automaticity and focus in neural processes underlying empathy.CORE NEURAL REGIONS FOR EMPATHYA essential purpose to look at empathy for many feelings below several different attentional situations is that it makes it possible for for an analysisof core neural regions for empathy. Earlier analysis has identified neural regions which might be regularly activated for the duration of empathy for physical pain (i.e., dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, dACC; and anterior insula, AI) (Morrison et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2005; Zaki et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2009; Lamm et al., 2011). These dependable activations within the dACC and AI have led some researchers to conclude that these regions are part of a core network in empathy (Fan et al., 2011). Nevertheless, it's unknown no matter whether the dACC and AI are necessary to empathic processes additional normally (i.e., not just empathy for pain) and whether or not these regions are activated through empathy for each optimistic and adverse feelings. Current neuroimaging investigation suggests that other neural regions--such because the [https://www.medchemexpress.com/GSK2656157.html GSK2656157 web] medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC; BA 10), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC; BA 9), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; BA 11)--may be involved in empathic processes.N Psychophysiology. Lewin, K. (1936). A., Bard, E. G. and Jeuniaux, P. (in press). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cogn. Sci. Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest ahead of trees: the precedence of worldwide attributes in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Little, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes in the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving together: toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Exp. Brain Res. 211, 329?36. Richardson, D.
Empathy enables us to know and share others' emotions, creating a bridge amongst the self plus the innermost experiences of an additional particular person.
+

Поточна версія на 12:44, 9 вересня 2017

Nadig, A., and Sedivy, J. (2002). Proof of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. GSK2656157 Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest ahead of trees: the precedence of international attributes in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Tiny, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes within the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving with each other: toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Even though previous study has recommended that specific factors--such as similarity to the target and familiarity with an experience--can trigger empathy (Preston and De Waal, 2002; Mitchell et al., 2006; Xu et al., 2009), really little investigation has examined how interest impacts our potential to empathize. Previous study suggests that empathy may happen instantaneously and automatically when we recognize another's emotional state (Preston and De Waal, 2002), even if we are cognitively busy. On the other hand, other research suggests that empathy is disrupted when we are distracted and cognitively occupied (Gu and Han, 2007). Simply because attentional sources are typically depleted during daily interactions, it is actually significant to understand if empathy is automatically engaged or calls for controlled and effortful processing. Thus, the present study examines the part of automaticity and focus in neural processes underlying empathy.CORE NEURAL REGIONS FOR EMPATHYA essential purpose to look at empathy for many feelings below several different attentional situations is that it makes it possible for for an analysisof core neural regions for empathy. Earlier analysis has identified neural regions which might be regularly activated for the duration of empathy for physical pain (i.e., dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, dACC; and anterior insula, AI) (Morrison et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2005; Zaki et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2009; Lamm et al., 2011). These dependable activations within the dACC and AI have led some researchers to conclude that these regions are part of a core network in empathy (Fan et al., 2011). Nevertheless, it's unknown no matter whether the dACC and AI are necessary to empathic processes additional normally (i.e., not just empathy for pain) and whether or not these regions are activated through empathy for each optimistic and adverse feelings. Current neuroimaging investigation suggests that other neural regions--such because the GSK2656157 web medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC; BA 10), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC; BA 9), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; BA 11)--may be involved in empathic processes.N Psychophysiology. Lewin, K. (1936). A., Bard, E. G. and Jeuniaux, P. (in press). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cogn. Sci. Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest ahead of trees: the precedence of worldwide attributes in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Little, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes in the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving together: toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Exp. Brain Res. 211, 329?36. Richardson, D.