The Best Way To Identify A Genuine Alisertib
Uniformity Some researchers (e.g., Stromswold, 2000; Guasti, 2002) have suggested that children acquire language in a very similar manner, going through the same stages at approximately the same ages, in spite of the fact that they are exposed to different input. Stromswold (2000), for instance, observes that ��Within a given language, the course of language acquisition is remarkably uniform��. Most children say their first referential words at 9 to 15 months�� and for the next 6-8 months, children typically acquire single words fairly slowly until they have acquired approximately 50 words��. Once children have acquired 50 words, their vocabularies often increase rapidly��. At around 18 to 24 months, children learning morphologically impoverished languages such as English begin combining words to form two-word utterances��. Children acquiring such morphologically impoverished languages gradually begin to use sentences longer than two words; but for several months their speech often lacks phonetically unstressed functional category morphemes such as determiners, auxiliary verbs, and verbal and nominal inflectional endings ��. Gradually, omissions become rarer until children are between three and four years old, at which point the vast majority of English-speaking children��s utterances are completely grammatical.�� (p. 910) This uniformity, Stromswold argues, indicates that the course of language acquisition is strongly predetermined by an innate program. There are several points to be made in connection with this argument. First, many of the similarities that Stromswold mentions are not very remarkable: we do not need UG to explain why children typically (though by no means always) produce single word utterances before they produce word combinations, or why frequent content words are acquired earlier than Alisertib function words. Secondly, the age ranges she gives (e.g., 9�C15 months for first referential words) are quite wide: 6 months is a very long time for an infant. Thirdly, the passage describes typical development, as evidenced by qualifiers like ��most children,�� ��typically,�� ��often����so the observations are not true of all children. Finally, by using qualifiers like ��within a given language�� and limiting her observations to ��children acquiring morphologically impoverished languages�� Stromswold implicitly concedes the existence of crosslinguistic differences. These are quite substantial: children acquiring different languages have to rely on different cues, and this results in different courses of development (Bavin, 1995; Jusczyk, 1997; Lieven, 1997); and they often acquire ��the same�� constructions at very different ages. For example, the passive is acquired quite late by English speaking children��typically (though by no means always��see below) by age 4 or 5, and even later��by about 8��by Hebrew-speaking children (Berman, 1985).