N Psychophysiology. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of Topological Psychology. New York, NY

Матеріал з HistoryPedia
Перейти до: навігація, пошук

Cogn. Sci. Metzing, C., and Brennan, S. (2003). When conceptual pacts are broken: partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions. J. Mem. Lang. 49, 201?13. Nadig, A., and Sedivy, J. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of worldwide options in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353?83. Norris, C. J., Chen, E. E., Zhu, D. C., Small, S. L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The interaction of social and emotional processes inside the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1818?829. Obhi, S. S., and Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving together: toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Exp. Brain Res. 211, 329?36. Richardson, D. C., and Dale, R. (2005). Planning to recognize: Empathy makes it possible for us to understand and share others' feelings, generating a bridge involving the self plus the innermost experiences of another particular person. Past study suggests that empathy may perhaps happen instantaneously and automatically when we recognize another's emotional state (Preston and De Waal, 2002), even though we are cognitively busy. On the other hand, other research suggests that empathy is disrupted when we're distracted and cognitively occupied (Gu and Han, 2007). Simply because attentional sources are usually depleted during daily interactions, it really is essential to understand if empathy is automatically engaged or calls for controlled and effortful processing. Thus, the current study examines the role of automaticity and interest in neural processes underlying empathy.CORE NEURAL REGIONS FOR EMPATHYA essential explanation to look at empathy for several emotions under various attentional situations is the fact that it allows for an analysisof core neural regions for empathy. Previous research has identified neural regions that are consistently activated through empathy for physical discomfort (i.e., dorsal 496791-37-8 anterior cingulate cortex, dACC; and anterior insula, AI) (Morrison et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2005; Zaki et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2009; Lamm et al., 2011). These reliable activations within the dACC and AI have led some researchers to conclude that these regions are a part of a core network in empathy (Fan et al., 2011). Having said that, it's unknown whether the dACC and AI are essential to empathic processes extra typically (i.e., not only empathy for discomfort) and no matter whether these regions are activated during empathy for each good and negative feelings. Recent neuroimaging study suggests that other neural regions--such as the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC; BA 10), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC; BA 9), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; BA 11)--may be involved in empathic processes.N Psychophysiology. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of Topological Psychology. New York, NY: McGraw Hill. Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R. A., Bard, E. G. and Jeuniaux, P. (in press). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cogn. Sci. Metzing, C., and Brennan, S. (2003). When conceptual pacts are broken: partner-specific effects around the comprehension of referring expressions. J. Mem. Lang. 49, 201?13. Nadig, A., and Sedivy, J. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychol. Sci. 13, 329?36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest ahead of trees: the precedence of Apabetalone biological activity international capabilities in visual perception.