Preposterous Arginase Insights And Ways It Could Very Well Impact You

Матеріал з HistoryPedia
Версія від 06:36, 15 червня 2017, створена Drawer9parade (обговореннявнесок) (Створена сторінка: This suggests that while there is some solid evidence for cross-linguistic sound-symbolism, ideophones are not completely intuitive to speakers of other languag...)

(різн.) ← Попередня версія • Поточна версія (різн.) • Новіша версія → (різн.)
Перейти до: навігація, пошук

This suggests that while there is some solid evidence for cross-linguistic sound-symbolism, ideophones are not completely intuitive to speakers of other languages. Developmental literature suggests that this general cross-linguistic insensitivity to ideophones may be conditioned in adults by language��s arbitrary and conventionalized connections. Children are more sensitive to sound-symbolic patterns in Japanese than to arbitrary forms, and this sensitivity appears to function as a scaffold for language acquisition (Ishiguro, 1993; Iwasaki et al., 2007b; Imai et al., 2008). This finding is consistent across both Japanese and English-speaking children (Kantartzis et al., 2011; Yoshida, 2012), providing evidence toward a cross-linguistic �C or, perhaps more accurately, language-independent �C early sensitivity toward sound-symbolism. EEG studies on sound-symbolism have found a variety of different effects at a variety of different time points, and so it is still unclear what the general effects of sound-symbolism are. Most EEG studies have used matching tasks, where participants are presented with images and sound-symbolic non-words which are either sound-symbolically congruent (e.g., kiki and a spiky shape) or incongruent. Kovic et al. (2010) found that congruent conditions elicited a greater negative-going wave at 140�C180 ms, and that this effect was most prominent at occipital electrodes. Asano et al. (2015) tested preverbal 11-months-old infants, and found that incongruent conditions elicited greater N400 responses. Moreover, phase synchronization of neural oscillations increased more in incongruent conditions around 400 ms, suggesting that incongruent conditions required sustained effort for cross-modal binding. They also found amplitude increases in the gamma band in centro-parietal within 300 ms of word onset for congruent conditions, suggesting that 11-month-olds process sound-symbolism as perceptual binding. Bien et al.��s (2012) study on cross-modal binding appears to be consistent with Asano et al.��s (2015) Arginase findings. Bien et al. (2012) investigated pitch-size mappings rather than linguistic sound-size mappings, and found that cross-modally congruent conditions (i.e., high pitch and small size) elicited a greater P2 response at around 250 ms in intra-parietal regions. Bien et al. (2012) argue that this effect is synaesthetic, and that cross-modal mappings underlie and influence multisensory perception. Taken together, the Bien et al. (2012) and Asano et al. (2015) studies suggest that cross-modally congruent conditions show an early effect in centro-parietal areas which is related to sensory processing, while incongruent conditions show a later effect which is related to semantic integration difficulty. The Kovic et al. (2010) study found a still earlier effect for congruent conditions, but at occipital regions, and moreover was a negative-going rather than positive-going wave. This may be related to the fact that Kovic et al.